Class Notes (834,244)
Canada (508,434)
Psychology (4,208)
PSY274H5 (129)
Lecture 9

PSY274 Lecture 9 (Nov 5).docx

10 Pages
112 Views
Unlock Document

Department
Psychology
Course
PSY274H5
Professor
Craig Chambers
Semester
Fall

Description
PSY274 Lecture 9 - Human communication & Advertising - Marketing communications o 1) communicating things about BRANDS  Building associations o 2) communicating things about specific PRODUCTS  “claims” about products  What advertisers say  How they say it - Background o What kinds of things are advertised and why? Where is it advertised? o Some products are not advertised or subjected to advertisements. - Examples of advertisements o Law o Cosmetics o Pharmaceutical products o TV shows o Cleaning products o Cars o Domains where there a lot of choice and the virtues are not inherent - What things are not usually advertised? o When differences are obvious - Historically, Macintosh did not have much advertisements because they were dominating a certain sector and businesses buy them for certain computers and functions - Top TV advertisers in US o Chart - Some things that were not advertised before o Water bottles o Razor blades  Different blades - Branding o Goal: build an ASSOCIATION between a brand and particular qualities/”feelings” o Examples  Apple  Technology, innovative  Goodyear  Pepsi  Popular, refreshing  Mercedes  Cool  Fido  Fun and young o Cultivate the association with the brand over time o In this class, we’re interested in linguistic things - Ways to link associations to brands… - Brand naming: o Revisiting arbitrariness:  Sound pattern for a given concept is generally arbitrary  BUT: some sounds or sound combinations seem to “fit” better than others for certain concepts - Examples o TIMEX for watches o Lettuce  Greenex lettuce  Farmex  Healthex  The examples above don’t seem like a good fit - Iconicity in speech sounds o Acoustic iconicity (onomatopoeic words)  Swoosh, crash, etc; o Articulatory iconicity  Rigid/soft things  Small/big things - Which one is the KIKI and which one is the BOORA? o “K” sound, interruption of air stream and then a release - What is the basis of the impression? o e.g. KIKI vs. BOORA o consonant sounds o Those that fully stop the flow of air: choppy airflow – jagged edges? o Those that do not fully impede airflow: smooth edges? - Another example: o Vowel sounds  bleef vs blawf - vowel sounds produced with comparatively narrowed oral cavity (e.g. “ee” sound)  little things - vowel sounds produced with comparatively open oral cavity (e.g. “aw” sound  big things - not the sound made, but the way the sound were made (what the mouth is doing) - associative iconicity (phonaesthemes) o e.g. sn- gl- fl-  sn- are usually refer to words that are related to the nose (sneer, sniff, snot)  gl- are usually refer to words that are related to optical property of light (glow, glimmer, glitter) o language specific, not in general o Overall implications: if people are sensitive to these patterns, brand naming should take them into account!  Ex. Don’t name a bridal company, Snoof o Another incidental source of “meaning” in brand names: The company a word keeps - Lexical neighbourhood (similar sounding words) o E.g. DARK  Dart, hark, darth, dork, darn, bark, Dirk, lark, Mark…  Hearing/reading a word may subconsciously “activate” meanings of similar sounding words - The Ford Edsel (late 1950s) o “Lexical neighbours”?  Pretzel, dead cell, hard sell… - Compare with: “Apple” o Applaud, application, appeal, ripple, (Snapple?)… o Conclusion: brand names (even made-up names!) can indirectly “inherit” meanings due to abstract SOUND-MEANING associations OR the meanings of SIMILAR-SOUNDING WORDS (can be both a good and bad thing for an advertiser - So far… o Many products/services not easily differentiable in terms of meaningful/objective features o Consequence for advertisers: must CREATE differences in mind of consumer o How? Create associations between product/brand and specific concepts relating to lifestyle/happiness/status/satisfaction, etc;  Language can be one means to do this o Next stop: PERSUASION - Background o Humans are COOPERATIVE language users o (E.g. turn-taking, common ground and perspective-taking, the balance between speaker economy and auditor economy, etc;) o This is also reflected in how we interpret the content of sentences: we routinely “fill in” information that is not explicitly communicated  Fred returned the book to the library  He must’ve took the book out before  Zelda is getting a divorce  She must be currently married  Valerie’s cell phone rang loudly in the middle of the lecture  She probably made an attempt to make it silent - This “filling in” process = inference (adding information beyond what is explicitly provided) o How robust is this process? o Examples from classic excerpts in cognitive psychology - Example in class - Loftus and Palmer (1974) o Showed film of two cars in an accident o Viewers estimates of speed where higher when less neutral terms such as “smashed” were used in question - Loftus, Miller and Burns (1978): o Did you see any blood? o When you saw the broken glass, did you see any blood?  There were no broken glass, but people usually accepts this and causes them to remember things in the scene that weren’t actually true - Loftus and Zanni (1975): o Did you see THE broken headlight  Most likely to say yes o Did you see A broken headlight  Most likely to say no, and that’s correct - Cooperative behaviour that humans have In which advertisers can draw on that to make advertisements more persuasive - Inferences in language – two major types o Presuppositions  The filled in/added information is something we “have to” assume in order for the sentence to actually make sense o Implications  The filled in/added information is optional and is added according to our assessment of what is plausible or relevant  There are typically many implicatures that could in principle be generated from a sentence, and the one(s) we choose tend to depend on context - PRESUPPOSITION o “Fred returned the book to the library”   someone must’ve taken a book out previously o “Zelda is getting a divorce”  She must be currently married o A “test” for presupposition  Follow up with a second sentence denying the information inferred in the first one. The result will sound contradictory  Fred returned the book to the library. But, the book was never taken out in the first place o Sounds strange, so you know presupposition exist in the first sentence and there was inference - IMPLICATURE o “Valerie’s cellphone rang loudly in the middle of lecture.”   she probably made some attempt to silence it o A “test” for implicature  Follow up with a second sentence denying the information inferred in the first one. The result may be surprising or sound odd, but will NOT be contradictory  E.g. Valerie’s cellphone rang loudly in the middle of the lecture. She didn’t even make an attempt to silence it. - Human communication = characterized by automatic process of “filling in” , in part with inferences - Advertisers capitalize on this by choosing language that is laden with inferences. A consequence is that the “persuasive” part of their message may be implicit rather than explicit o Good for bypassing the need to be able to support your claims - Presupposition in advertising language o Examples from reading  We’re cutting prices AGAIN  Infer that it’s not the first time that prices were cut  Don’t forget to come check
More Less

Related notes for PSY274H5

Log In


OR

Join OneClass

Access over 10 million pages of study
documents for 1.3 million courses.

Sign up

Join to view


OR

By registering, I agree to the Terms and Privacy Policies
Already have an account?
Just a few more details

So we can recommend you notes for your school.

Reset Password

Please enter below the email address you registered with and we will send you a link to reset your password.

Add your courses

Get notes from the top students in your class.


Submit