Class Notes (1,100,000)
CA (630,000)
UTSG (50,000)
PSY (4,000)
PSY100H1 (1,000)
Lecture 9

PSY100H1 Lecture Notes - Lecture 9: Prototype Theory, Ludwig Wittgenstein, Wags


Department
Psychology
Course Code
PSY100H1
Professor
Connie Boudens
Lecture
9

This preview shows page 1. to view the full 5 pages of the document.
Lecture 9 - 03-18-13
Chapter 8; Concepts and Generic Knowledge
Lecture Outline:
definitions
prototypes and typicality effects
exemplars
difficulties with categorizing via resemblance
concepts as theories
definitions
when we have a concept like dog or chair, we take it for granted that this should be something
easily understood. Defining these concepts are more complex than our first glance. These are
the building blocks of all the information that we know. It's the building blocks of our general
knowledge. We start by knowing what the dog or chair is.
dog: mammal with 4 legs, barks, wags its tail. There's examples that are exceptions to this rule.
If you saw a 3 legged dog, you would still know that it's a dog, or a dog that doesn't bark. If you
can always have an exception to the definition, then how are we defining these terms in which
we believe are so simplistic?
This philosopher, Ludwig Wittgenstein had this idea that simple concepts have no definition. If
you try to define them, there's no real way of defining them. Example: game, played by children,
for fun, some type of rules, involves multiple people, done during leisure time; we can think of
exceptions to what a game is.
Gambling is also a game. Playing games for fun can also be professional sports. There are rules
in some games but not something like Lego. There are things that involves multiple people, but
solitaire is a game but it's individual. Competition could be a possibility but tea party is also an
exception.
The way that we can start to think about this, rather than saying that there are definitions, we
can think of family resemblance. Guy with the beard, glasses, etc., that's the ideal member of
the family. There are also atypical members. Maybe we do look like a brother, sister, mother or
father. Maybe same thing for your cousins, may have overlapping features.
Dog probably has 4 legs, barks and wags its tail. Creatures without these feature are unlikely to
be a dog. There's an ideal member that is a dog with these traits and there are members of the
concept dog that don't necessarily fit that in which we still recognize as a dog but is not likely to
happen.
The more characteristic feature an object has, the more likely we are to believe it is part of the
category. If we see a random animal run by, may have heard a bark, 4 legs, probably a dog. How
many of these characteristics actually fit the correct description of a dog?
Prototypes and typicality effects
prototypes
You're Reading a Preview

Unlock to view full version

Only page 1 are available for preview. Some parts have been intentionally blurred.

We have prototypes, ideal members are prototypes of that concept. Prototypical dog is German
sheppard, other dogs are less prototypical yet they still fit a lot of that prototype in which you
can generalize to. That prototype possesses all features when we think of a dog for instance.
This differs amongst individuals and cultures. We can say that having this prototype is based on
what we mostly see often. If you grow up in a particular part of the word with specificities, it'll
be different than other parts of the world. What your prototype is will be different. You're
averaging out all the ones that you basically see. If you're living in Canada, you have specific
prototypes. Exposure to more examples of that house changes your general idea of what your
ideal house would be.
We can say that prototypes have fuzzy boundaries. Because of the way that they are set, there's
no clear boundaries in which a certain thing is defined as a prototype or not. If you were looking
at the red squares, which are the best red, studies have shown that people have similar
believes. It seems odd to do this, the fact is that people can still can do that task. There's
something different about as being a best example of a red despite all the squares are all
typically red. In the text, it talks about odd and even numbers. You can have a number and it fits
into one of those categories. Let's say you're given a bunch of numbers and to have to judge
which are "evener", people can still rate them on different levels and so there's something
about this that kind of brings the point that maybe there's better examples of the prototypes in
other exemplars that fit into that concept.
Most of these items are somewhat implicit. It's more or less than when you see these things
growing up, it builds a conceptual base of a web of information in your head and becomes a
type of activity in which you distinguish.
Sentence-variation task
Robins are birds, penguins are birds, people are slower at defining the latter. You have to take a
second think whether or not it fits your prototype of a bird. It's not that you consciously thought
about the fit. When people respond faster to something like this, you're saying that the example
that fits has more characteristics in common with the prototype that the individual has.
Production tasks
These typicality effects can also be seen when we give participants production tasks. Example:
name as many birds as you can. They'll start out with items that are more typical with their
experience with birds (more common) and as you move down the list, they become more and
more atypical.
Does this picture show you a phone? The first one is an iphone, if you have more experience
with that, it becomes quicker. The latter is the older phone, you can still recognize that but is a
slower response, it's more temporal because this generation is more used to the typical phone
that resembles something like the iphone. It fits that particular individual prototype in which is
shaped by where you are, and your experiences.
We also talk about privilege memberships. Category members are more privileged if they're
closer to your prototypes. The chart is about the fruits and birds. Rank these on how well/bird-
like/fruit-like they are. Apples and pears and grapes and strawberries are rated quite high, quite
typical. Pumpkins are less fruit like. Same thing with birds, bats, wings, size of a bird.
You're Reading a Preview

Unlock to view full version